Resource Theory and Mindless Theory represent two of the most
prominent theories in the study of vigilance. A relatively more recent theory,
namely Goal Habituation theory, has also attracted much attention in this
field. This week’s post will examine these theories theories in detail,
beginning with a summary of the main points of each theory and a comparative
analysis of the key similarities and distinctions. As a departure from previous
posts, this week’s post will not feature any in-depth summaries of the papers,
but will instead reference them throughout this post for evidence and
discussion. In the questions and comments section, we will then integrate this
week’s readings with those of previous weeks by comparing the debates between
mindless and resource theory with those discussed in previous weeks,
specifically those concerning the key contributors to the vigilance decrement. After
an examination of the competing theories, we will select the most compelling
theory by referencing evidence from this week’s readings. Lastly, we will
conclude with comments on links to neuroscience and possible future directions.
COMPETING THEORIES
AND COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS
Resource Theory
The central tenet of resource theory is the contention that
vigilance requires limited processing
resources. As Helton and
Russell (2011) and Ariga
et. al. (2011) elaborated in their papers,
resource theory maintains that vigilance tasks require subjects to make
continuous discriminations and thus deplete
their mental and cognitive resources, resulting in a decline in their performance efficiency. Notable supporters of resource theory include
Parasurman and Warm, whose vigilance taxonomy and review of advances in
vigilance research we discussed last week. Thus, advocates of resource theory
espouse the view that the vigilance
decrement is due to the decline in “available attention resources” and
“cognitive resources available for information-processing” (Helton and
Russell, 2011). The robustness of resource theory lies primarily in the abundance of supportive evidence coming
from a variety of different experiments, from behavioral, to brain imaging, to
mental workload studies, as Helton and
Russell (2012) details in their paper. For instance, See et. al. showed objective increases in
task difficulty was directly linked to greater lapses of sustained attention.
Furthermore, declines in cerebral blood flow in areas of the brain associated
with vigilance have also been shown to correspond with the vigilance
decrement. Ultimately, the resource
theory posits that the vigilance decrement
is a manifestation of the depletion of mental and cognitive resources
throughout the course of a vigil.
Mindless Theory
In contrast to resource theory, mindless theory holds that the vigilance decrement arises from the
subjects’ inattention or mindless. In short, the subjects demonstrate
decreased attention to the vigilance task out of boredom. First described by Robertson and colleagues, the mindless
theory states that subjects’ “supervisory
attentional system loses effectiveness and ceases to focus awareness on the
vigilance task” after a certain time of performing the same, repetitive
task. As a result, subjects begin to respond to the task in a “thoughtless
manner,” and acts out of routinization rather than attention (Ariga and Lleras,
2011). Since the Mindless Theory is relatively younger than Resource Theory,
supportive evidence is slightly more limited. Much of the evidence for mindless
theory comes from results of sustained attention to response task (SART)
studies. Robertson
et. al. (1996) describes this
approach as well as its advantages over the conventional perceptual detection-based
tests such as the Triplets test. Specifically, SART involves withholding of key presses in response
to signals. The rationale behind this paradigm was the view that SART tasks require a “high level of
continuous attention” and thus would test uniquely for sustained attention. Results
confirmed this hypothesis and indicated that SART was sensitive to vigilance
deficits and “predicts reported attentional failures in subjects.” Analysis of
SART indices of attentional disengagement (measured through three variables,
namely response time speed, anticipations, and omissions of signal responses)
have shown a correlation between the vigilance decrement and the subject’s
attentional disengagement, thus providing evidence for the mindless theory.
Goal Habituation Theory
The
goal habituation theory, though similar to the Mindless Theory, has several
important differences with the latter. Proposed by Ariga and Lleras (2011),
this new theory attributes the vigilance decrement to the difficulty in maintaining an active goal for a “prolonged period of
time.” Specifically, they argue that cognitive
goals, such as maintaining attention through a vigilance task, also show
habituation effects (described as the “eventual absence of representation
of sustained stimulation”), and that it is this “goal habituation” that accounts for the decrement in vigilance tasks. In
accordance with this theory, Ariga and Lleras argued “deactivating the
vigilance goal would preempt full goal habituation by re-strengthening the goal’s
activation,” and thus manifest in an enhancement in vigilance. Results
indicated that the “mere prompt” of activating a different goal was sufficient
to enhance vigilance, a finding that clearly supports the habituation
hypothesis.
QUESTIONS AND COMMENTS
Blast From the Past:
Parallels and Distinctions Between Current and Previous Debates
Examination of these competing theories brings to mind clear
parallels of the debates we discussed in previous weeks on the key cause of the
vigilance decrement. For instance, while signal detection theory held the
background event rate responsible, Broadbent and Gregory attributed it to the
decline in subject confidence. Upon a closer look, we find that the debate
between resource, mindless, and goal habituation theories has clear echoes of
the past debates. For instance, resource theory maintains the vigilance
decrement is due to a depletion of mental resources, which is rather in line
with Parasurman’s argument that memory load contributes to sensitivity
decrements. To take this one step further, resource theory’s claim that
cognitive and mental resources are depleted also echo See et. al.’s study on the necessity to examine the vigilance decrement
in the context of the total task
demands, in addition to memory load. Furthermore, the cognitive component
addressed by resource theory is also in line with See et. al. and other researchers’ view that vigilance tasks require
cognitive resources and that cognitive-based tasks may be different from
perceptual-based tasks. Interestingly, however, while See et. al. saw a stabilization of performance and in some cases, a
rise in performance, during cognitive tasks, resource theory does not take such
observations into consideration. On another note, the Mindless Theory also
falls in line with several of Scerbo (1998)’s findings. Specifically, the
latter posited that boredom could have an effect in the vigilance decrement,
which is clearly echoed in the Mindless Theory’s claims that the vigilance
decrement arises out of inattention to the vigilance task. On a more “meta”
level, I find the current debates and those that we discussed last week all
share one similarity that may explain the existence of the debates: they all
hypothesize that one factor or process is the key contributor to the vigilance
decrement. However, as will be discussed in the section immediately following
this one, the ideal theory should incorporate components of all the candidate
theories.
Moment of Truth: Which
Theory is Most Compelling (and Why)?
Okay, so here, I’m going to err…cheat a bit. I know we’re
technically suppose to select one theory, but I don’t think there’s one theory
that is clearly the winner. Moreover, I feel that quite possibly, the “correct
theory,” is a mixture of all three theories. I believe the most compelling
theory should have the evidence (and claims) of resource theory but also take
into consideration the possibilities suggested by mindless and goal habituation
theory. For something as complex as vigilance, I don’t think that any one
variable or explanation could be the only correct explanation. To do so would
be to neglect the complexity of the problem as well as that of the evidence. The
key, at least in my opinion, is to recognize that the theories presented are not mutually exclusive. For me, the
most compelling theory might look something like this: the depletion of mental
and cognitive resources (which is proposed by resource theory) can be due to both mental fatigue and boredom, which may stem from goal habituation. While this is
rudimentary, it takes into account many variables such as resource depletion
and mental fatigue but also links together possible explanations into one
compelling argument. Oh, and disclaimer: I’m not self-complimenting, really.
Well, maybe just a tiny bit.
General Questions and
Comments
In this section, I would like to
raise a general question that I have wondered about for a long time. It does
not address this week’s papers specifically, per se, but this week’s assignment
rekindled this question. I know the scientific method is the bible of science,
and that everything begins with observation and a hypothesis. However, I feel
that once we believe that something is true/i.e. have a hypothesis, we are
almost certain to find evidence to support it. In short, it is possible to
basically prove anything as long as we look for evidence in favor of our
“hypothesis.” Yet, doesn’t this contribute to biased data and results? Aside
from undermining the rigorous nature of science research, if this approach
indeed biases our findings (and by virtue of being human, we all have
preferences, etc.), and if that research is then used to produce clinically
relevant products, isn’t this a significant problem? Clearly, I think the
scientific method has significant merits. I’m just wondering about a possible
flaw. I agree that most of this can be controlled by discipline, but sometimes,
I feel that maybe starting out with no hypothesis but rather with the intention
of studying something, may be more compelling. Just a thought though.
LINKS TO NEUROSCIENCE
I
think it would be extremely interesting to examine the competing theories
through the lens of neuroscience experiments. For resource theory, we would
need to first define what constitutes “mental and cognitive resources.” We
could begin by testing just a few variables such as memory and attention. We
can then construct mouse models in which the mutant mice have impaired ability
to process information or tire more quickly. If the resource theory is correct,
then one would expect the mutant mice to demonstrate a quicker and more
pronounced vigilance decrement than the wild type mice. A difficulty that has
to be overcome is pinpoint the exact manipulation that would result in impaired
information processing. One approach is legion of the hippocampus or frontal
cortex. One could also use RNAi to silence genes that are known to be important
in attention and information processing. For the mindless model, we can use
human subjects. Specifically, we can use fMRI and transcranial brain imaging to
see which areas of the brain are activated during repetitive and “boring”
tasks, and most importantly, monitor the decline in activation intensity over
the course of the vigil. To incorporate goal habituation theory, we can perform
a similar procedure, but insert goal-changes sporadically. If the theory is
correct, then one would expect to see in increase in activation of
vigilance-related brain areas (every time a goal change is promoted) following
a decline in those areas after a prolonged time.
REFERENCES
Robertson,
I. H., T. Manly, et al. (1997). 'Oops!':
performance correlates of everyday
35(6):
747-758.
Allan
Cheyne, J., G. J. Solman, et al. (2009).
Anatomy of an error: a bidirectional state
111(1):
98-113.
Helton,
W. S. and P. N. Russell (2011). Feature
absence-presence and two theories of
lapses of
sustained attention. Psychol Res 75(5): 384-392.
Ariga,
A. and Lleras, A. (2011). Brief
and rare mental ‘‘breaks’’ keep you focused:
118:439–443.
Helton,
W. S., & Russell, P. N. (2012). Brief mental
breaks and content-free cues may not
keep you
focused. Experimental Brain Research, 219, 34–46.
Image
References:
Graduate Studies in Neuroscience (Msc/PhD) http://www.uleth.ca/artsci/neuroscience/graduate-studies-neuroscience-mscphd.
(Accessed 13/02/2013).
Eco-Cinema and Film Genre. The Land Before Time and
Evolutionary Narratives. http://ecocinema.blogspot.com/2012/06/land-before-time-and-evolutionary.html.
Accessed 13/02/2013)
No comments:
Post a Comment