Wednesday, February 27, 2013

EXECUTIVE FAILURE VS. EXECUTIVE FUNCTION


Mind wandering is a common phenomenon in everyone’s daily lives. Thinking about lab during molecular biology class. Thinking about this blog post during antibody incubations. Thinking about sleep at 5:30am…okay, this might be due to fatigue. Yet, the exact mechanisms that account for mind wandering remains inconclusive. However, there are two prominent and competing views on mind wandering, namely the “executive function” and the “executive failure” views of mind wandering. In this post, we will examine both viewpoints in detail, beginning with a description of the key components of each viewpoint, followed with a comparative analysis on the similarities and conflicts between the two camps. Later on, in a questions and comments section, we discuss the evidence for and challenges against each theory. Upon consideration of both evidence and challenges, we proceed to choose the most compelling theory. Finally, we conclude with a section on links of these two theories to neuroscience and possible future experiments.

Summary and Comparative Analysis  


Before we delve into a discussion of the evidence for and challenges against the two models of mind wandering, it is important to first establish what the “executive function” and “executive failure” models entail. Thus, below, we provide a description of both theoretical viewpoints, including key components of each theory. Subsequently, we examine the fundamental conflict between these two viewpoints in a comparative analysis.

Executive Function

Conceived and detailed by Smallwood and Schooler in 2006, the “Executive Function” model argues that mind wandering utilizes executive resources and thus reduces the amount of executive resources available for the primary task at hand (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006; McVay and Kane, 2010). This view falls into the “resource theory” category in that it espouses the view that there exists a limited pool of executive resources responsible for information processing and attention (and lack thereof). As Smallwood et. al. notes, “When mind wandering occurs, the executive components of attention appear to shift away from the primary task, leading to failures in task performance and superficial representations of the external environment.” Importantly, the executive function viewpoint also maintains that mind wandering “can be seen as a goal-driven process, albeit one that is not directed toward the primary task” (Smallwood, 2006) and thus performs an executive function.

Executive Failure

In response to Smallwood and Schooler’s 2006 study, McVay and Kane proposed an alternative model on mind wandering that generally agrees with but extends upon Watkins’ control theory. Specifically, they argue mind wandering is a result of “a failure of executive control over automatically (and continuously) cued thoughts rather than as consuming executive resources.” (McVay and Kane, 2010) in their “control failure x concerns perspective” on mind wandering. In direct contrast to Smallwood and Schooler, McVay and Kane contends that mind wandering “does not draw on the same executive resources as executive control,” and thus can be controlled or prevented using the “executive–control system.” Ultimately, this viewpoint holds that mind wandering reflects a failure in executive control and serves no useful function.

Comparative Analysis: The Conflict and the Conversation

I found this week’s papers particularly interesting because they responded directly to each other and thus allowed for a conversation on the key questions in each paper. Thus, in this comparative analysis, we will examine the key points in this conversation.  The conflict between these two viewpoints is clear: one theory holds mind wandering consumes executive resources and may serve functional roles in daily life whereas the other model contends that mind-wandering is a result of executive failure and does not consume the same executive resources as the primary task. At first glance, this seems like a lost case; how can you ever reconcile two completely opposite viewpoints? Smallwood (2010) again stepped up to the task in his “Reply to McVay and Kane.” In response to MacVay and Kane’s contention that mind wandering is a result of control failure, Smallwood argues that despite the possibility of mind wandering happening after control failure, it nevertheless is “always a conscious reportable experience and so is globally available to the system.” He continues to argue that this global availability suggests that mind wandering demands resources, in accordance with his executive-function viewpoint. The most striking point in this conversation was not the different viewpoints, but their potential semi-coexistence. Specifically, I was surprised that Smallwood (2010) defended his viewpoint by introducing a completely new concept, namely global availability. What I found most compelling about Smallwood’s reply was the consistency of his “global availability hypothesis” with the key features of McVay and Kane’s model. However, as will be addressed in the next section, I found the concept and introduction of the global availability hypothesis somewhat arbitrary. Even more concerning, I found the assumptions associated with the “global workspace” quite severe.


Questions and Comments


The Evidence and Challenges

There exists robust evidence both for and against these two theories on mind wandering. Let’s begin with the Executive Function theory elaborated by Smallwood and Schooler. If mind wandering does indeed utilize executive resources, then this model would predict an inverse correlation between mind wandering and performance on the primary task. This is exactly what several studies found. As the authors note, “the evidence presented in this section suggests trade-offs between mind wandering and task performance.” Specifically, mind wandering was also inversely correlated with the difficulty of the primary task, which would also support a resource theory approach. If mind wandering utilizes executive resources, and assuming that more difficult tasks exhaust more resources, tasks that are more difficult and thus demand more resources would leave less available resources for mind wandering. Even more pressingly, evidence exists to show that mind wandering actually competes for working-memory resources; specifically, impairments (slight, but present) in task efficiency are observed in the presence of mind wandering. The observation that mind wandering interferes with the “successful completion of nonautomated task” suggests that mind wandering requires working memory and executive resources.
However, although evidence clearly exists to support the executive function viewpoint, I have several concerns with the way in which the evidence was collected and the way mind wandering is defined. For instance, much of the results discussed here rely on self-reports and questionnaires. This runs into the exact same problem that we discussed in last week’s blog post, namely a lack of consistency. Without a rigorous definition of mind-wandering, the authors of these studies risk invalidating their results. Specifically, subjects can choose to not report TUITs or may not catch slight instances of TUITs. Furthermore, the conception of what constitutes “mind wandering” may be different among subjects, which serves as a further possible confounder of the results. Furthermore, I found the meta-analysis of the evidence a bit troubling. Although I know that this happens very often in research and can be very enlightening, I think it is important to have normalization procedures in place when one does such an analysis. For instance, Smallwood cites evidence from several different times of studies, from SART studies to text comprehension. However, he did not dwell upon the possibility that evidence from across such different studies may require further processing before they could be compared to each other.
In reply to Smallwood’s 2006 paper, McVay and Kane (2010) provides evidence for a control failure model of mind wandering. In accordance with this theory, mind wandering should be prevented when “control is proactively initiated and maintained” in response to task demands. Importantly, they distinguish between two types of executive control, namely proactive and reactive, that are dissociable by behavioral measures and association with different brain areas. The majority of the evidence for McVay and Kane stems from studies done utilizing individuals with differences in their propensity to mind wander. Specifically, the authors provide what they believe to be counter-evidence for Smallwood and Schooler (2006)’s executive function theory. They argue that if mind wander indeed demands executive resources, then individuals with more resources (namely high working-memory-capacity individuals) should exhibit more mind wandering than low working-memory-capacity individuals. However, they found the opposite effect: subjects with higher WMC clearly demonstrated greater executive control and less frequent mind wandering across tasks. The authors argue that this evidence serves to counteract the executive function resource theory presented by Smallwood and Schooler (2006). Furthermore, the authors cite the default network to provide evidence for their executive failure theory. The default network is essentially a collection of different regions in the brain are implicated in mind wandering (found to be especially active during mind wandering). The authors reference the neuroimaging results from Weissman et. al. (2006), which found trade-offs between control areas of the brain and the default network. Specifically, reductions in PFC activation, which was interpreted to be reductions in attention-control area activity, “reliably predicted lapses of attention” (McVay and Kane, 2010). The authors cite this “anti-correlation” between default network activation and executive network activation as evidence for their view that executive control failures are responsible for mind wandering.
There are several clear problems with the strategy presented above. Specifically, the association of the prefrontal cortex with exclusively proactive executive control and the anterior cingulate cortex with exclusively reactive executive control provide an oversimplification of the functions of the PFC and ACC. Although the association may definitely be present, there is no definite engram, and no particular region of the brain is exclusively responsible for one task. Furthermore, there are clear alternative explanations for PFC activation that were not considered in the studies mentioned. Thus, one cannot make any decisive conclusion without proper controls, which were also not described in the papers. Furthermore, I found the association of working memory capacity (WMC) with executive resource capacity very troubling. McVay and Kane (2010) essentially utilize WMC as a proxy for the amount of executive resources an individual has at his/her disposal. Although working memory capacity definitely counts as an executive resource, it is not the only such resource, and a higher working memory capacity does not necessarily imply the existence of greater executive resources. Lastly, I found the authors’ discussion of the default network as evidence particularly troubling. Again, the utilization of PFC activation as a proxy for executive control without the proper controls impinges severely upon the validity of their data. Furthermore, the conclusion they drew from the results was also ungrounded. Indeed, the trade off between “executive” areas of the brain and the default network could provide evidence for the very hypothesis McVay and Kane (2010) are trying to disprove, namely the executive function hypothesis. One can interpret this observed “trade off” as an indication that mind wandering utilizes executive resources, thus leading to lower activation rates in the executive areas of the brain (corresponding to a higher level of activation in the default network). Thus, the evidence presented has clear flaws.

The Choice



After evaluating Smallwood’s original resource theory centered executive function viewpoint, McVay and Kane (2010)’s evidence, and Smallwood’s reply, I must say that I am leaning towards the executive function viewpoint. However, this is only in light of the evidence presented for each theory and the qualifications the respective authors mentioned. On a psychological level, however, I don’t think either theory really tells the whole story. Rather, I think a hybrid of these two theories would make for a complete description of mind wandering. Executive function and executive failure models need not be completely mutually exclusive, as Smallwood implied in his 2010 paper. Just as different types of mind wandering exist (recall Giambra defined at least two: directed mind wandering and involuntary mind wandering), I believe different mechanisms behind mind wandering also exist. For instance, executive function may account for mind wandering during certain conditions, such as those in which mind wandering is beneficial for adaptation or creativity purposes, whereas mind wandering could be due to executive failure in other situations. I would be very interested to see further investigation into this topic matter and especially the introduction of a hybrid theory.

Links to Neuroscience
 


Okay, after my rant on McVay and Kane (2010)’s assumptions in their neuroimaging evidence, it’s clear that I think it’s very difficult to biologically evaluate these two theories. Don’t get me wrong, though; I think neuroimaging provides extremely valuable data. The key is in the interpretation of the neuroimaging results; correlation studies are both insightful and conducive to future studies. However, one just needs to be careful to not form causal relationships on the basis of correlational studies. In addition to neuroimaging studies, however, I think it would be interesting to have a mouse model of these two viewpoints. For instance, the researcher could create two separate populations of mouse, with the experimental group being deficient in executive resources. One way to accomplish this could be silencing a key pathway that is known to be important in information processing. Furthermore, one could also create a third group of mice with working memory deficits. We can then train these mice to perform a specific task, and task efficiency could be evaluated across populations to see if a difference exists between mice with access to different degrees of executive resources. Furthermore, we could also compare the WMC deficient mice with the executive resource deficient mice to see if the former’s task efficient accurately predicts that of the latter. However, several hurdles must be first overcome before this could be realized. For one, a pathway that is critical to information processing needs to be defined and results need to be accurately controlled for. Given the appropriate controls and experimental design, I think this study could be very enlightening!

References




Image References



No comments:

Post a Comment