Vigilance and Sustained Attention: What Is Vigilance and What Accounts for Its Decay?
Vigilance,
or sustained attention, is a phenomenon that plays an essential role in our
daily lives. The requirement for attention is ubiquitous—in school, work,
relationships, and just about every aspect of life. Naturally, sustained
attention—vigilance—has become a topic of extreme interest to many researchers.
In today’s post, we examine the work of three research groups on vigilance and
the factors that influence it. We begin with a summary of the key points of each group’s work, with a particular
focus on the main experimental questions
posed, key results obtained, interpretations of those results, and inferences made. After this review of
results, we perform a comparative
analysis of these studies to determine whether the researchers’ views are compatible. We are interested in the
similarities and points of departures in the three groups’ results and
interpretations. A deeper analysis will follow, in which we examine whether the researchers’ inferences are
justified; here, we will raise questions concerning the experimental design
and interpretation of results as well as point out possible alternative
interpretations. Lastly, partially because I find this topic intriguing and
partially because I couldn’t really resist as a neuroscience junkie, we will
briefly explore the links between the
psychological study of vigilance and neuroscience before concluding with
some remarks on possible future
directions.
Overview/Summary of Data and Results
Jerison and Pickett
In
their 1964 paper on “Vigilance:
The Importance of the Elicited Observing Rate,” Jerison and Pickett took a
decision-theory based approach to answering the question “what causes vigilance
to degrade, as measured by the decreased ability to detect rare signals in a
‘prolonged vigil’” (1964). Specifically, the authors argue that “a failure of
attention” is to blame for this phenomenon; furthermore, they attribute this
failure of attention to the “observer’s decision about whether or not to
observe.” As the authors note in the paper, this approach implies that the
demand on observing behavior is more important than the rate of the signals
presented. To test their hypothesis, Jerison and Pickett tested subjects under
two conditions: in one, subjects were asked to detect signals against a series
of slowly-occurring (5 times/minute) background events in a time period of 80
minutes. In the other, the subject were asked to do the same thing against a
series of faster-occurring background events (30 times/minute). In direct
contrast to the arousal hypothesis, they found that subjects in the first group
(with the low background event rate) only missed about 10% of the signals,
whereas the subjects in the second group (with high background event rate)
missed about 70% of the signals. Since variables such as memory and time were
controlled for in this study, Jerison and Pickett inferred from their results
that “the observer’s decision about whether or not to observe is associated
with the average payoff or ‘expected value’ of attending to the regularly
recurring events.” Specifically, the authors inferred that the observer choose
to behave most economically, and thus are more likely to attend and have higher
vigilance when the probability of detecting the signals is high (during low
background event rate), and less likely to attend when the probability is low,
such as during high background event rate.
Broadbent and Gregory
Broadbent and Gregory’s 1965
paper investigated the effects of noise and signal rate on vigilance, a
phenomenon that was analyzed through the lens of decision theory. More
specifically, they address the question whether changes in vigilance are due to
changes in the evidence the operator uses to make decisions or changes in the
operator’s criteria in making and reporting such decisions. To approach this
question, Broadbent and Gregory considered two variables: d’, which they defined
as a measurement of the strength of the signal (and thus independent of the
operator’s criterion), and beta, which is defined as a measure of the
operator’s “degree of caution” in doubtful situations. The authors tested
subjects by asking them to report signal observation with a “yes,” “no,” or
“maybe.” The consideration of doubt in their study is relatively novel and
departs from previous studies such as that of Jerison and Picket. Interestingly,
contrary to previous findings by Mackworth and Taylor, the authors of this
paper found that there was no decrease in d’ over time, though a rise in beta
was noted. Thus, subjects became more wary of reporting signal observation as
time went on. Furthermore, they found high levels of noise increased the
subjects’ confidence while low levels of noise was correlated with lower
confidence. The authors inferred from this result that the operator’s criteria
for a signal become more strenuous as a function of time. Furthermore, the
authors also inferred from their results that at low signal rates, the operator
is less willing to report uncertain signals, whereas the opposite phenomenon is
observed at high signal rates. Thus, the authors attribute a possible decrease
in vigilance to a reduction in the sensitivity of the observer as a function of
time during the vigilance task.
Scerbo
In
his 1998 paper on “What’s
So Boring About Vigilance,” Scerbo performed a series of studies in which
he attempted to induce boredom via utilizing the vigilance boredom. These
studies represented the first endeavor at studying boredom experimentally and
were performed with the following objectives: to elucidate the nature of
boredom and to determine the role of task-associated factors and personality in
the perception of boredom. We will provide a brief summary of the key points
and findings of the four experiments that Scerbo performed to this end.
Scerbo’s initial experiment was a test of O’Hanlon
(1981)’s theory of boredom, which held that boredom is a psychophysiological
state that occurs when the optimal arousal level necessary for a given task is
unable to be maintained. Specifically, he argues that boredom is created by the
stress associated with “fighting habituation.” Thus, it follows that more
demanding tasks should yield higher levels of boredom. To test this theory,
Scerbo had subjects observe a display for white lines that increased in
conspicuity, as increases in conspicuity have been positively correlated to
signal detection. Boredom was assessed with a variation of Barmack’s method.
Interestingly, although perpetual sensitivity was higher for high conspicuity
signals, boredom ratings were similar for both low and high conspicuity
conditions, a result that is inconsistent
with O’Hanlon’s original theory. Additionally, Scerbo also found that the
onset of boredom occurs very quickly in vigilance tasks, usually peaking after
10 minutes.
Scerbo’s second study investigated the validity of using
movement level as a proxy for the subject’s degree of boredom. In accordance
with results of previous studies, Scerbo hypothesized that movement was
directly proportional to a subject’s degree of boredom. Thus, the more bored a
subject is, the more movement he/she should exhibit during the vigil. In this
study, movement was assessed by the extent to which subjects moved a mouse
cursor during the vigil. Results were in accordance with his hypothesis; he
found subjects who moved the mouse the most also reported the greatest levels
of boredom. Furthermore, he also found subjects who reported the greatest
boredom also had the highest “mental workload” in vigilance tasks and thus had
the worst performance. Scerbo made several inferences from the results of this
study; specifically, he inferred that subjects who find vigilance “particularly
boring” may not be good candidates for sustained attention tasks; furthermore,
he suggests that the high mental workload experienced during vigilance may be
associated with the boring nature of the task.
Scerbo’s third study examined the effects of stimulus
variety and satiation on boredom. Subjects were presented with two tasks: one
conventional vigilance task and a kaleidoscope task, in which various
“exciting” stimuli (kaleidoscope-like screen saver) were presented and subjects
asked to press a key when the pattern was composed of predominately white
lines. In accordance with their hypothesis, Scerbo et. al. found subjects reported lower degrees of boredom for the
kaleidoscope task than for the vigilance task. As for satiation, Scerbo
reasoned that giving subjects the option to quit during a vigilance task should
significantly decrease the level of reported boredom, as subjects can quit when
the task becomes overly boring. Interestingly, however, he found mixed results;
while most subjects quit before the experimental duration, most subjects still
reported feelings of boredom
In Scerbo’s fourth and final experiment, he investigated
the effect of personality factors in the perception of boredom. In short, he
found that subjects had varying levels of boredom proneness (BP), and
individuals with higher BP reported greater levels of boredom after a vigilance
task. Furthermore, he also found that subjects with high BP were sensitive to
instruction manipulation whereas subjects with low BP were largely unaffected. From
the collective results of these studies, Scerbo inferred that vigilance “can
indeed be quite boring.” Furthermore, subjects can feel boredom very strongly
and minor changes in task-related parameters may be insufficient to relieve
boredom. Additionally, individual-specific factors, such as boredom proneness
vary among subjects and should be taken into consideration in vigilance tasks.
Importantly, individuals with great BP would most likely make for poor test
subjects. Scerbo concludes with his belief that it may be important to continue
studying the source of boredom in vigilance.
Comparative Analyses of Studies
The
authors for today were all interested in the paradigm of vigilance and
performed studies in attempts to elucidate factors that impact vigilance and
contribute to the degradation of vigilance over time. Although there exist
several similarities in their views, there are also multiple important
distinctions. Here, we comparatively examine the authors’ views and ultimately
suggest that are only compatible to a certain degree. For instance, Jerison and
Pickett considered only one variable in their study of vigilance, and as they
noted in their paper, background event rate was credited with being the major
determinant in the decay of vigilance. Broadbent and Gregory performed a
similar study on vigilance, and although they took a decision theory-based
approach to their study (similar to Jerison and Pickett), they went one step
further and took into account the existence of doubt in the subjects’ reports.
Their conclusion, namely that the effects seen during a low-signal rate
vigilance task are in part due to a “reduced willingness to report uncertain
signals” (1965), is compatible with Jerison and Pickett’s findings, but
provides an alternative explanation for the latter’s results. Thus, while the
two research groups come to a similar conclusion, their interpretations of that
conclusion are different. Scerbo’s series of studies on boredom in vigilance
contributes a further viewpoint to the discussion on the decay of vigilance over
time. For instance, the results of his experiments indicate that boredom can be
overwhelming for subjects and can significantly affect their performance in
vigilance tasks. He goes on further to suggest that individuals with
particularly high boredom proneness would make for poor test subjects in
vigilance studies, as they are almost intrinsically primed to do poorly. Are
Scerbo’s viewpoints incompatible with those of Jerison and Pickett and
Broadbent and Gregory? Certainly not. But they’re not completely compatible
either. Scerbo raises an important question, namely the role of boredom in
vigilance and its source, that the two previous research groups did not
consider. One can think of the overarching question of what accounts for the
decay of vigilance as a mosaic, and each author’s work as specific pieces of
that mosaic. By itself, no one author or research group provides the complete
view. Together, however, they raise interesting questions and challenge each
other to help construct a better view of the mosaic.
A Deeper Analysis: Questions and Comments
Now
that we have reviewed the authors’ key findings and discussed the compatibility
of their viewpoints, it is worthwhile to examine their inferences in detail and
determine whether they’re justified. Furthermore, we will also point out
alternative interpretations of results and possible confounding factors. Let’s
begin with Jerison and Pickett. Although their experiments were properly
controlled for the most part and their results indicative of their inferences,
they failed to include an adequate sample size for their vigilance tasks. Since
their study only had 20 subjects, the trends seen in their results are less
than compelling. One can question whether their results are representative of
the actual phenomenon. What if there were a few subjects with particularly high
boredom proneness (a factor that was not even considered in this study)? While
the gender and memory of the subjects were controlled, their personalities were
not, and this could skew the results, especially with such a low sample size.
Furthermore, aside from these factors, the results obtained by Jerison and
Pickett could be accounted for by alternative explanations in addition to the
background signal rate theory. For instance, aspects such as the degree of fatigue
of individuals prior to commencing the vigilance task and their tendency to
become bored would be important factors to consider.
Broadbent
and Gregory improved upon Jerison and Pickett’s study on vigilance by
considering the element of doubt during each reported decision. Although I felt
their results were more comprehensive than those of Jerison and Pickett, I
still found some of there inferences partially unjustified. For instance, their
conclusion that a reduced willingness to report signals during doubtful situations
in low-signal conditions definitely has merit and is supported by the results,
but is also not the only possible explanation of the data. Fatigue is a
possible physiological explanation. It is possible that a third variable such
as fatigue was contributing to the decrease in confidence and thus the decrease
in vigilance. This may also be compatible with their finding on the
relationship between noise and confidence. Audio stimuli such as noise may
bring the individual to a higher level of arousal, which may veil some of the
effects of fatigue. Consequently, the subjects’ fatigue levels may be decreased
in the presence of noise, which would correlate with an increased confidence.
I
found Scerbo’s studies of boredom in vigilance extremely fascinating as one of
the first studies to examine boredom experimentally and investigate the
presence and role of boredom in vigilance. However, I was bothered by the fact
that Scerbo never really provides a definition for boredom and what constitutes
boredom. He rejects O’Hanlon’s definition of boredom, but does not provide a
discrete definition himself. Although he provides several ways of measuring
boredom (Barmack’s method, etc.), he does not provide a way to normalize
reports of boredom. As he himself notes in his report, individuals have
different boredom proneness; thus subjects may have different perceptions of
what constitutes boredom. Without a normalization scheme, the reports used in
his study remain rather subjective. The reliance of his inferences were based
on such subjective reports may introduce an element of doubt into the level of
justification of his inferences.
The
largely qualitative nature of the research groups’ experiments is also a
possible area of concern. Clearly, it is extremely difficult to quantify
something nebulous such as boredom and thus boredom assays and similar assays
must rely heavily on subject responses. However, these responses may not be
normalized, and one person’s idea of “extreme boredom” or an “8/9” may be very
different from that of a different subject. Furthermore, all three studies
employ visual tasks in the
assessment of vigilance. However, sustained attention is required in several
non-visual tasks. For instance, is the vigilance required in an oral interview
a different sort of vigilance? If the subjects were subjected to auditory tasks
in the assessment of vigilance, would the results be the same?
Links to Neuroscience and Possible Future Directions
I confess that I’m
obsessed with neuroscience. So I was really interested in the biological and
chemical differences in brains of a subject at rest, under sustained attention,
and during boredom. I think it would be interesting to monitor which areas of
the brain are activated during vigilance tasks in real time; more specifically,
I would be interested to see the differences in activation patterns between the
beginning and middle of the vigilance trail. A rudimentary experimental design
could be as follows: subjects could be placed in an fMRI machine and shown the
visual stimuli associated with the vigilance task. They can then be asked to
verbalize a response every time a signal is detected. This experiment may shed
light on the neurobiological mechanisms underlying sustained attention as well
as a biological explanation for the decay of vigilance. Once we have a better
idea of this, we could expand to a more systematic/network level by
investigating the pathways and neural networks that contribute to vigilance and
sustained attention.
Thanks
for reading! And I’m really sorry that it’s too long—I got a little carried
away.
References
Broadbent, D.E.
and Gregory, M. (1965). Effects
of Noise and Signal Rate Upon Vigilance Analysed by Means of Decision Theory.
Human Factors, 7, 155-162.
Jerison, H.J.
and Pickett, R.M. (1964). Vigilance:
The Importance of the Elicited Obseving Rate. Science, 143, 970-971.
Scerbo,
M. (1998). What’s
So Boring About Vigilance? In R.B. Hoffman, M.F. Sherrick, & J.S. Warm
(Eds.), Viewing Psychology As A Whole: The Integrative Science of William N.
Dember (pp. 145-166). Washington, DC: APA.
Image References:
Question Marks. http://www.servitokss.com/question-marks/. 31/1/2013/
Study Shows Effects of Combat on Brain. http://vetlawyers.com/vetblog/index.php/2011/02/study-shows-effects-of-combat-on-brain/. 31/1/2013.
Study Habits Cartoons and Comics. CSL Cartoonstock. http://www.cartoonstock.com/directory/s/study_habits.asp. 31/1/2013.
Image References:
Question Marks. http://www.servitokss.com/question-marks/. 31/1/2013/
Study Shows Effects of Combat on Brain. http://vetlawyers.com/vetblog/index.php/2011/02/study-shows-effects-of-combat-on-brain/. 31/1/2013.
Study Habits Cartoons and Comics. CSL Cartoonstock. http://www.cartoonstock.com/directory/s/study_habits.asp. 31/1/2013.
No comments:
Post a Comment